Try a new search

Format these results:

Searched for:

person:ms149

Total Results:

20


Cognition and behavioral changes occur prior to subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) in the evolution of brain aging and alzheimer's disease (AD): >25 years prior to the advent of mild dementia [Meeting Abstract]

Reisberg, B; Shulman, M; Torossian, C; Monteiro, I; Boksay, I; Gill, T; Chakraborty, R; Zeeshan, M; Zhu, W
EMBASE:70807767
ISSN: 0893-133x
CID: 174188

Outcome over seven years of healthy adults with and without subjective cognitive impairment

Reisberg, Barry; Shulman, Melanie B; Torossian, Carol; Leng, Ling; Zhu, Wei
BACKGROUND: Subjective cognitive impairment (SCI) in older persons without manifest symptomatology is a common condition with a largely unclear prognosis. We hypothesized that (1) examining outcome for a sufficient period by using conversion to mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia would clarify SCI prognosis, and (2) with the aforementioned procedures, the prognosis of SCI subjects would differ significantly from that of demographically matched healthy subjects, free of SCI, termed no cognitive impairment (NCI) subjects. METHODS: A consecutive series of healthy subjects, aged > or =40 years, presenting with NCI or SCI to a brain aging and dementia research center during a 14-year interval, were studied and followed up during an 18-year observation window. The study population (60 NCI, 200 SCI, 60% female) had a mean age of 67.2 +/- 9.1 years, was well-educated (mean, 15.5 +/- 2.7 years), and cognitively normal (Mini-Mental State Examination, 29.1 +/- 1.2). RESULTS: A total of 213 subjects (81.9% of the study population) were followed up. Follow-up occurred during a mean period of 6.8 +/- 3.4 years, and subjects had a mean of 2.9 +/- 1.6 follow-up visits. Seven NCI (14.9%) and 90 SCI (54.2%) subjects declined (P < .0001). Of NCI decliners, five declined to MCI and two to probable Alzheimer's disease. Of SCI decliners, 71 declined to MCI and 19 to dementia diagnoses. Controlling for baseline demographic variables and follow-up time, Weibull proportional hazards model revealed increased decline in SCI subjects (hazard ratio, 4.5; 95% confidence interval, 1.9-10.3), whereas the accelerated failure time model analysis with an underlying Weibull survival function showed that SCI subjects declined more rapidly, at 60% of the rate of NCI subjects (95% confidence interval, 0.45-0.80). Furthermore, mean time to decline was 3.5 years longer for NCI than for SCI subjects (P = .0003). CONCLUSIONS: These results indicate that SCI in subjects with normal cognition is a harbinger of further decline in most subjects during a 7-year mean follow-up interval. Relevance for community populations should be investigated, and prevention studies in this at-risk population should be explored
PMCID:3873197
PMID: 20129317
ISSN: 1552-5260
CID: 107277

Commentary on "a roadmap for the prevention of dementia II: Leon Thal Symposium 2008." Subjective cognitive impairment as an antecedent of Alzheimer's dementia: policy import [Editorial]

Reisberg, Barry; Shulman, Melanie B
PMCID:2951836
PMID: 19328449
ISSN: 1552-5279
CID: 100593

Steroid dementia: a follow-up [Comment]

Sacks, Oliver; Shulman, Melanie
PMID: 17310040
ISSN: 0028-3878
CID: 641962

Steroid dementia: An overlooked diagnosis?

Sacks, O; Shulman, M
The authors studied a 72-year-old man with polymyalgia rheumatica who, after taking 100 mg of prednisone for 3 months, developed a psychosis followed by dementia. It was initially considered that the dementia was a separate neurodegenerative condition, probably of Alzheimer type, but when steroids were discontinued, he rapidly returned to his previous level of functioning. Reviewing the literature regarding the effects of steroids on cerebral function, the authors found that such cases of 'reversible dementia' are not uncommon, although rarely given the emphasis they deserve. The authors believe, given the extensive use of steroids in medical practice, that physicians should be more aware of this important cause of reversible dementia
ISI:000227206800024
ISSN: 0028-3878
CID: 48916

Laterality and psychopathology circa 1976 [Comment]

Shulman, Melanie B
PMID: 14527503
ISSN: 1525-5050
CID: 42655

Treatment of memory disorders in epilepsy

Shulman MB; Barr W
Impaired memory is a common and often debilitating complaint in patients with epilepsy. Overlapping variables such as seizure control, attentional dysfunction, and mood disorders further complicate diagnosis and management. Direct therapy for memory deficits associated with epilepsy is rarely attempted. The varied pharmacological (AED selection, cholinesterase inhibitors, stimulants, antidepressants, and herbal supplements) and nonpharmacological approaches to cognitive remediation in epilepsy patients are reviewed
PMID: 12609318
ISSN: 1525-5069
CID: 95532

The clinical applications of EEG: origins

Shulman MB
PMID: 12609339
ISSN: 1525-5050
CID: 39288

Triangular backgrounds shift the bias of line bisection performance in hemispatial neglect

Shulman, M B; Alexander, M P; McGlinchey-Berroth, R; Milberg, W
OBJECTIVE: Patients with left neglect on line bisection show normal implicit sensitivity to manipulations of both the stimulus and the visual background. Three experiments were designed to define this sensitivity more exactly. METHODS: Normal controls and patients with left neglect performed a series of horizontal line bisection tasks. Independent variables were the configurations of the backgrounds for the line-rectangle, square, circle, left and right pointing isosceles triangles-and whether the background was the shape of the piece of paper or an outline drawn on a standard piece of paper. In a separate experiment different components of the triangle were outlined on a piece of paper. Deviation from true midpoint was calculated. RESULTS: Simply placing the target lines in a symmetric background such as a square or circle did not reliably reduce neglect. A triangle asymmetric in the horizontal plane caused a shift in bisection away from the triangle's vertex. With right pointing triangles the perceived midpoint shifted to the left of true centre (crossed over). The effects of the triangles were comparable in the patients and the controls when controlled for baseline bisection bias. The critical components of the triangles were the angular legs. This effect of background was not influenced by lesion site or by hemianopia. CONCLUSIONS: Patients with left visual neglect remain sensitive to covert manipulations of the visual background that implicitly shift the perceived midpoint of a horizontal line. This effect is strong enough to eliminate neglect on a bisection task. The mechanism of this effect is expressed through preattentive visual capacities
PMCID:1737718
PMID: 11784828
ISSN: 0022-3050
CID: 26512

The Frontal Lobes, Epilepsy, and Behavior

Shulman MB
The frontal lobes have been overshadowed by the temporal lobes in the vast literature addressing the neurobehavioral and psychological perspectives of epilepsy. The purpose of this review is to summarize contemporary anatomicobehavioral correlations and to highlight the frontal lobe contributions to the neurology, neuropsychology, and neuropsychiatry of epilepsy, in general, and to temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) and frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE), in particular. Much evidence has accumulated suggesting that focal epileptogenic tissue may have effects on distant neural systems. Data supporting the case that the frontal regions are preferentially affected in TLE are presented. Emphasis is placed on the results of numerous functional imaging studies demonstrating correlations between frontal hypoperfusion and cognitive or mood impairments in patients with TLE
PMID: 12737828
ISSN: 1525-5050
CID: 63728