Try a new search

Format these results:

Searched for:

in-biosketch:yes

person:ghilam01

Total Results:

157


Discrete and continuous planning of hand movements and isometric force trajectories

Ghez, C; Favilla, M; Ghilardi, M F; Gordon, J; Bermejo, R; Pullman, S
We have previously demonstrated that, in preparing themselves to aim voluntary impulses of isometric elbow force to unpredictable targets, subjects selected default values for amplitude and direction according the range of targets that they expected. Once a specific target appeared, subjects specified amplitude and direction through parallel processes. Amplitude was specified continuously from an average or central default; direction was specified stochastically from one of the target directions. Using the same timed response paradigm, we now report three experiments to examine how the time available for processing target information influences trajectory characteristics in two-degree-of-freedom forces and multijoint movements. We first sought to determine whether the specification of force direction could also take the form of a discrete stochastic process in pulses of wrist muscle force, where direction can vary continuously. With four equiprobable targets (two force amplitudes in each of two directions separated by 22 degrees or 90 degrees), amplitude was specified from a central default value for both narrow and wide target separations as a continuous variable. Direction, however, remained specified as a discrete variable for wide target separations. For narrow target separations, the directional distribution of default responses suggested the presence of both discrete and central values. We next examined point-to-point movements in a multijoint planar hand movement task with targets at two distances and two directions but at five directional separations (from 30 degrees to 150 degrees separation). We found that extent was again specified continuously from a central default. Direction was specified discretely from alternative default directions when target separation was wide and continuously from a central default when separation was narrow. The specification of both extent and direction evolved over a 200-ms time period beginning about 100 ms after target presentation. As in elbow force pulses, extent was specified progressively in both correct and wrong direction responses through a progressive improvement in the scaling of acceleration and velocity peaks to the target. On the other hand, movement time and hand path straightness did not change significantly in the course of specification. Thus, the specification of movement time and linearity, global features of the trajectories, are given priority over the specific values of extent and direction. In a third experiment, we varied the distances between unidirectional target pairs and found that movement extent is specified discretely, like direction, when the disparity in distances is large. The implications of these findings for contextual effects on trajectory planning are discussed. The independence of extent and direction specification and the prior setting of response duration and straightness provide critical support for the hypothesis that point-to-point movements are planned vectorially
PMID: 9224851
ISSN: 0014-4819
CID: 142679

Learning a visuomotor transformation in a local area of work space produces directional biases in other areas

Ghilardi, M F; Gordon, J; Ghez, C
1. The dependence of directional biases in reaching movements on the initial position of the hand was studied in normal human subjects moving their unseen hand on a horizontal digitizing tablet to visual targets displayed on a vertical computer screen. 2. When initial hand positions were to the right of midline, movements were systematically biased clockwise. Biases were counterclockwise for starting points to the left. Biases were unaffected by the screen location of the starting and target positions. 3. Vision of the hand in relation to the target before movement, as well as practice with vision of the cursor during the movement, temporarily eliminated these biases. The spatial organization of the biases suggests that, without vision of the limb, the nervous system underestimates the distance of the hand from an axis or plane that includes its most common operating location. 4. To test the hypothesis that such an underestimate might represent an adaptation to a local area of work space or range effect, subjects were trained to reach accurately from right or left positions. After training, movements initiated from other locations, including ones that were previously error free, showed new biases that again represented underestimates of the distance of the initial hand position from the new trained location. 5. We conclude that hand path planning is dependent on learned representations of the location of the hand in the work space
PMID: 7666158
ISSN: 0022-3077
CID: 142680

Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception

Sainburg, R L; Ghilardi, M F; Poizner, H; Ghez, C
1. We recently showed that patients lacking proprioceptive input from their limbs have particular difficulty performing multijoint movements. In a pantomimed slicing gesture requiring sharp reversals in hand path direction, patients showed large hand path distortions at movement reversals because of failure to coordinate the timing of the separate reversals at the shoulder and elbow joints. We hypothesized that these reversal errors resulted from uncompensated effects of inertial interactions produced by changes in shoulder joint acceleration that were transferred to the elbow. We now test this hypothesis and examine the role of proprioceptive input by comparing the motor performance of five normal subjects with that of two patients with large-fiber sensory neuropathy. 2. Subjects were to trace each of six template lines presented randomly on a computer screen by straight overlapping out-and-back movements of the hand on a digitizing tablet. The lines originated from a common starting position but were in different directions and had different lengths. Directions and lengths were adjusted so that tracing movements would all require the same elbow excursion, whereas shoulder excursion would vary. The effects of varying interaction torques on elbow kinematics were then studied. The subject's dominant arm was supported in the horizontal plane by a low-inertia brace equipped with ball bearing joints and potentiometers under the elbow and shoulder. Hand position was monitored by a magnetic pen attached to the brace 1 cm above a digitizing tablet and could be displayed as a screen cursor. Vision of the subject's arm was blocked and the screen cursor was blanked at movement onset to prevent visual feedback during movement. Elbow joint torques were calculated from joint angle recordings and compared with electromyographic recordings of elbow joint musculature. 3. In control subjects, outward and inward paths were straight and overlapped the template lines regardless of their direction. As prescribed by the task, elbow kinematics remained the same across movement directions, whereas interaction torques varied substantially. The timing of the onsets of biceps activity and the offsets of triceps activity during elbow flexion varied systematically with direction-dependent changes in interaction torques. Controls exploited or dampened these interaction torques as needed to meet the kinematic demands of the task. 4. In contrast, the patients made characteristic errors at movement reversals that increased systematically across movement directions. These reversal errors resulted from improper timing of elbow and shoulder joint reversals.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
PMID: 7760137
ISSN: 0022-3077
CID: 142681

Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. II. Effects of visual information on accuracy

Ghez, C; Gordon, J; Ghilardi, M F
1. The aim of this study was to determine how vision of a cursor indicating hand position on a computer screen or vision of the limb itself improves the accuracy of reaching movements in patients deprived of limb proprioception due to large-fiber sensory neuropathy. In particular, we wished to ascertain the contribution of such information to improved planning rather than to feedback corrections. We analyzed spatial errors and hand trajectories of reaching movements made by subjects moving a hand-held cursor on a digitizing tablet while viewing targets displayed on a computer screen. The errors made when movements were performed without vision of their arm or of a screen cursor were compared with errors made when this information was available concurrently or prior to movement. 2. Both monitoring the screen cursor and seeing their limb in peripheral vision during movement improved the accuracy of the patients' movements. Improvements produced by seeing the cursor during movement are attributable simply to feedback corrections. However, because the target was not present in the actual workspace, improvements associated with vision of the limb must involve more complex corrective mechanisms. 3. Significant improvements in performance also occurred in trials without vision that were performed after viewing the limb at rest or during movements. In particular, prior vision of the limb in motion improved the ability of patients to vary the duration of movements in different directions so as to compensate for the inertial anisotropy of the limb. In addition, there were significant reductions in directional errors, path curvature, and late secondary movements. Comparable improvements in extent, direction, and curvature were produced when subjects could see the screen cursor during alternate movements to targets in different directions. 4. The effects of viewing the limb were transient and decayed during a period of minutes once vision of the limb was no longer available. 5. It is proposed that the improvements in performance produced after vision of the limb were mediated by the visual updating of internal models of the limb. Vision of the limb at rest may provide configuration information while vision of the limb in motion provides additional dynamic information. Vision of the cursor and the resulting ability to correct ongoing movements, however, is considered primarily to provide information about the dynamic properties of the limb and its response to neural commands
PMID: 7714578
ISSN: 0022-3077
CID: 142682

Impairments of reaching movements in patients without proprioception. I. Spatial errors

Gordon, J; Ghilardi, M F; Ghez, C
1. This paper introduces a series of studies in which we analyze the impairments in a planar reaching task in human patients with severe proprioceptive deficits resulting from large-fiber sensory neuropathy. We studied three patients, all of whom showed absence of discriminative tactile sensation, position sense, and stretch reflexes in the upper extremities. Muscle strength was normal. We compared the reaching movements of the patients with those of normal control subjects. The purpose of this first paper was no characterize the spatial errors in these patients that result primarily from impairments in the planning and execution of movement rather than in feedback control. This was done by using a task in which visual feedback of errors during movement was prevented. 2. Subjects were instructed to move their hand from given starting positions of different targets on a horizontal digitizing tablet. Hand position and targets were displayed on a computer screen. Subjects could not see their hand, and the screen display of hand position was blanked at the signal to move. Thus visual feedback during movement could not be used to achieve accuracy. Movement paths were displayed as knowledge of results after each trial. 3. Compared with controls, the patients made large spatial errors in both movement direction and extent. Directional errors were evident from movement onset, suggesting that they resulted from improper planning. In addition, patients' hand paths showed large curves and secondary movements after initial stops. 4. The overall control strategy used by patients appeared the same as that used by controls. Hand trajectories were approximately bell shaped, and movement extent was controlled by scaling a trajectory waveform in amplitude and time. However, both control subjects and patients showed systematic errors in movement extent that depended on the direction of hand movement. In control subjects, these systematic dependencies of extent on direction were small, but in patients they produced large and prominent errors. Analysis of the hand trajectories revealed that errors were associated with differences in velocity and acceleration for movements in different directions. In an earlier study, we showed that in subjects with normal sensation that the dependence of acceleration and velocity on direction results from a failure to take the inertial properties of the limb into account in programming the initial trajectory. In control subjects, these differences in initial acceleration are partially compensated by direction-dependent variations in movement time.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
PMID: 7714577
ISSN: 0022-3077
CID: 142683

Effects of drug manipulations on anterior components of somatosensory evoked potentials in a parkinsonian animal model

Onofrj, M; Ferracci, F; Fulgente, T; Malatesta, G; Ghilardi, M F
Somatosensory Evoked Potentials (SEPs) to median nerve stimuli were recorded in seven Cynomolgus monkeys before and after the induction of the MPTP-parkinsonian syndrome. SEPs recorded after the onset of parkinsonism showed a significant amplitude reduction of an anterior negative component peaking at about 15 ms (N15), independent of the severity of symptoms. The amplitude decrease was not reversed by the administration of I-dopa, despite clinical improvement, or cholinergic, noradrenergic and gabaergic agents. Amplitudes of N15 and of parietal P15 components were increased by the administration of the N-MDA antagonists ketamine and MK 801, and markedly increased when monkeys were given the anaesthetic agent etomidate. The present study shows that the reduced N15 SEP component of parkinsonian monkeys is similar to the reduced frontal N30 SEP component evidenced by other authors in patients affected by Parkinson's disease. The attenuation of N15 is not related to deficitary dopaminergic, noradrenergic, cholinergic and gabaergic systems. The implications of this finding and the role of glutamate toxicity are discussed
PMID: 7924892
ISSN: 0378-6501
CID: 142684

Accuracy of planar reaching movements. I. Independence of direction and extent variability

Gordon, J; Ghilardi, M F; Ghez, C
This study examined the variability in movement end points in a task in which human subjects reached to targets in different locations on a horizontal surface. The primary purpose was to determine whether patterns in the variable errors would reveal the nature and origin of the coordinate system in which the movements were planned. Six subjects moved a hand-held cursor on a digitizing tablet. Target and cursor positions were displayed on a computer screen, and vision of the hand and arm was blocked. The screen cursor was blanked during movement to prevent visual corrections. The paths of the movements were straight and thus directions were largely specified at the onset of movement. The velocity profiles were bell-shaped, and peak velocities and accelerations were scaled to target distance, implying that movement extent was also programmed in advance of the movement. The spatial distributions of movement end points were elliptical in shape. The major axes of these ellipses were systematically oriented in the direction of hand movement with respect to its initial position. This was true for both fast and slow movements, as well as for pointing movements involving rotations of the wrist joint. Using principal components analysis to compute the axes of these ellipses, we found that the eccentricity of the elliptical dispersions was uniformly greater for small than for large movements: variability along the axis of movement, representing extent variability, increased markedly but nonlinearly with distance. Variability perpendicular to the direction of movement, which results from directional errors, was generally smaller than extent variability, but it increased in proportion to the extent of the movement. Therefore, directional variability, in angular terms, was constant and independent of distance. Because the patterns of variability were similar for both slow and fast movements, as well as for movements involving different joints, we conclude that they result largely from errors in the planning process. We also argue that they cannot be simply explained as consequences of the inertial properties of the limb. Rather they provide evidence for an organizing mechanism that moves the limb along a straight path. We further conclude that reaching movements are planned in a hand-centered coordinate system, with direction and extent of hand movement as the planned parameters. Since the factors which influence directional variability are independent of those that influence extent errors, we propose that these two variables can be separately specified by the brain
PMID: 7925800
ISSN: 0014-4819
CID: 142685

Accuracy of planar reaching movements. II. Systematic extent errors resulting from inertial anisotropy

Gordon, J; Ghilardi, M F; Cooper, S E; Ghez, C
This study examines the source of direction-dependent errors in movement extent made by human subjects in a reaching task. As in the preceding study, subjects were to move a cursor on a digitizing tablet to targets displayed on a computer monitor. Movements were made without concurrent visual feedback of cursor position, but movement paths were displayed on the monitor after the completion of each movement. We first examined horizontal hand movements made at waist level with the upper arm in a vertical orientation. Targets were located at five distances and two directions (30 degrees and 150 degrees) from one of two initial positions. Trajectory shapes were stereotyped, and movements to more distant targets had larger accelerations and velocities. Comparison of movements in the two directions showed that in the 30 degrees direction responses were hypermetric, accelerations and velocities were larger, and movement times were shorter. Since movements in the 30 degrees direction required less motion of the upper arm than movements in the 150 degrees direction, we hypothesized that the differences in accuracy and acceleration reflected a failure to take into account the difference in total limb inertia in the two directions. To test this hypothesis we simulated the initial accelerations of a two-segment limb moving in the horizontal plane with the hand at shoulder level when a constant force was applied at the hand in each of 24 directions. We compared these simulated accelerations to ones produced by our subjects with their arms in the same position when they aimed movements to targets in the 24 directions and at equal distances from an initial position. The magnitudes of both simulated and actual accelerations were greatest in the two directions perpendicular to the forearm, where inertial resistance is least, and lowest for movements directed along the axis of the forearm. In all subjects, the directional variation in peak acceleration was similar to that predicted by the model and shifted in the same way when the initial position of the hand was displaced. The pattern of direction-dependent variations in initial acceleration did not depend on the speed of movement. It was also unchanged when subjects aimed their movements toward targets presented within the workspace on the tablet instead of on the computer monitor. These findings indicate that, in programming the magnitude of the initial force that will accelerate the hand, subjects do not fully compensate for direction dependent differences in inertial resistance.(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 400 WORDS)
PMID: 7925785
ISSN: 0014-4819
CID: 142686

The effect of levo-acetyl-carnitine on visual cognitive evoked potentials in the behaving monkey

Antal, A; Bodis-Wollner, I; Ghilardi, M F; Glover, A; Mylin, L; Toldi, J
We studied acute and chronic effects of levo-acetyl-carnitine (LAC) on event-related potentials (ERPs) in 3 monkeys trained in a 'go'/'no-go' visual 'oddball' discrimination task. The stimuli were 2.5 cpd sinusoidal gratings differing in their respective orientation only (0 degrees or 45 degrees). Each monkey was trained to release a lever during a prespecified time window. Target stimulus presentation probabilities were between 0.25 and 0.5. ERPs had comparable mean latencies and amplitudes in all monkeys. Primary evoked potentials recorded to either the target or non-target stimulus did not change significantly as a result of LAC treatment. On the other hand, P300 latency decreased following LAC administration, with a maximum occurring in 15-20 min. The major effects of LAC were consistent within each animal and for all three of them
PMID: 7682930
ISSN: 0013-4694
CID: 142687

Visual 'cognitive' evoked potentials in the behaving monkey

Glover, A; Ghilardi, M F; Bodis-Wollner, I; Onofrj, M; Mylin, L H
Using a visual 'oddball' paradigm we studied ERPs in monkeys trained in a 'go' 'no-go' discrimination task. The stimuli were 2.5 cpd sinusoidal gratings differing only in orientation (0 degrees or 25 degrees). Monkeys released a lever during 1 of 2 response windows (RW), 480-1762 or 740-1672 msec, following target stimulus onset. Target stimulus presentation probabilities were 1.0, 0.5 and 0.3. The primary evoked potentials recorded to either the target or non-target stimulus were similar in all monkeys. P3 signals progressively emerged in the monkeys only to the target stimulus. P3 recorded at Cz, P3, and P4 had similar mean latencies and amplitudes. Eye movements showed no relationship to P3 potentials. Neither the primary visual potentials nor P3 changed significantly as a function of RW. P3 amplitude was inversely related to target probability. When the target stimulus was presented 100% of the time (P = 1.0) P3 disappeared over 4-5 blocks of trials, while the primary evoked potentials remained consistent
PMID: 1703952
ISSN: 0013-4694
CID: 142688