Searched for: in-biosketch:yes
person:pg3
Where is the 7±2 category bottleneck?
Pelli, Denis; Majaj, Nay; Glimcher, Paul
It is well known that observers classifying signals varying along a single sensory dimension can only distinguish 7±2 categories (Miller, 1956). We wondered whether eye movements could escape this apparently cognitive bottleneck. In a dark room, the observer fixated a point of light, which was suddenly displaced horizontally by a random offset, and extinguished 150 ms later. The dual task required the observer to shift gaze to the brief target and to then verbally report the offset in cm. Amplitude of the saccadic eye movement was recorded by a scierai search coil. Preliminary results are surprising. On the one hand, the eye movements of both observers are no more accurate than their verbal reports. On the other hand, only the verbal reports showed the expected increase in precision when the stimulus range was reduced from 10-30 deg to 17.5-22.5 deg.
SCOPUS:33749217874
ISSN: 0146-0404
CID: 4188432
Response properties of saccade-related burst neurons in the central mesencephalic reticular formation
Handel, A; Glimcher, P W
We studied the activity of saccade-related burst neurons in the central mesencephalic reticular formation (cMRF) in awake behaving monkeys. In experiment 1, we examined the activity of single neurons while monkeys performed an average of 225 delayed saccade trials that evoked gaze shifts having horizontal and vertical amplitudes between 2 and 20 degrees . All neurons studied generated high-frequency bursts of activity during some of these saccades. For each neuron, the duration and frequency of these bursts of activity reached maximal values when the monkey made movements within a restricted range of horizontal and vertical amplitudes. The onset of the movement followed the onset of the burst by the longest intervals for movements within a restricted range of horizontal and vertical amplitudes. The range of movements for which this interval was longest varied from neuron to neuron. Across the population, these ranges included nearly all contraversive saccades with horizontal and vertical amplitudes between 2 and 20 degrees. In experiment 2, we used the following task to examine the low-frequency prelude of activity that cMRF neurons generate before bursting: the monkey was required to fixate a light-emitting diode (LED) while two eccentric visual stimuli were presented. After a delay, the color of the fixation LED was changed, identifying one of the two eccentric stimuli as the saccadic target. After a final unpredictable delay, the fixation LED was extinguished and the monkey was reinforced for redirecting gaze to the identified saccadic target. Some cMRF neurons fired at a low frequency during the interval after the fixation LED changed color but before it was extinguished. For many neurons, the firing rate during this interval was related to the metrics of the movement the monkey made at the end of the trial and, to a lesser degree, to the location of the eccentric stimulus to which a movement was not directed.
PMID: 9325383
ISSN: 0022-3077
CID: 199332
Responses of intraparietal neurons to saccadic targets and visual distractors
Platt, M L; Glimcher, P W
Current evidence suggests that neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area (LIP) reflects sensory-motor processes, but it remains unclear whether LIP activation participates directly in the planning of future eye movements or encodes data about both sensory events and the behavioral significance of those sensory events. To examine this issue, 31 intraparietal neurons were studied in awake, behaving monkeys trained to perform two tasks that independently controlled the location of a saccadic target and the location and behavioral relevance of a visual distractor. In both of these tasks, two eccentric light-emitting diodes (LEDs) were illuminated yellow, one above and one below a fixation stimulus. Shortly after the eccentric LEDs were illuminated, a change in the color of the fixation stimulus indicated which of these LEDs served as the saccadic goal and which served as a visual distractor. In the first or distractor-irrelevant task, fixation offset indicated that the subject must initiate a saccade shifting gaze to the saccadic goal. In the second or distractor-relevant task, distractor offset served as the saccade initiation cue. Intraparietal neurons responded more strongly in association with an LED that served as a saccadic target than in association with the same LED when it served as a visual distractor. Neuronal responses in association with either target or distractor stimuli on distractor-relevant and distractor-irrelevant blocks of trials were statistically indistinguishable. When the location of either the target or the distractor was varied across trials, the response of each neuron in association with a particular stimulus location was always greater for targets than for distractors and the magnitude of this response difference was independent of distractor relevance; however, distractors were nearly always associated with some intraparietal neuronal activity. A target/distractor selectivity index was computed for each neuron as the difference between responses associated with targets minus responses associated with distractors divided by the sum of these values. When the selectivity of each neuron on the distractor-relevant task was plotted against the selectivity of the same neuron on the distractor-irrelevant task, activity in the population of intraparietal neurons was found to be independent of distractor relevance. These data suggest that LIP neuronal activation represents saccadic targets and, at a lower level of activity, visual distractors, but does not encode the relevance of distractor stimuli on these tasks.
PMID: 9310444
ISSN: 0022-3077
CID: 199342
Where is the 7+/-2 category bottleneck? [Meeting Abstract]
Pelli, D; Majaj, N; Glimcher, P
ISI:A1997WN18603005
ISSN: 0146-0404
CID: 2754822
Do area LIP neurons encode the probability that a stimulus will serve as a saccadic target? [Meeting Abstract]
Platt, ML; Glimcher, PW
ISI:A1997WN18602142
ISSN: 0146-0404
CID: 2754812
Stimulus location probability regulates spatial attention [Meeting Abstract]
Cameron, EL; Glimcher, PW
ISI:A1997WN18601715
ISSN: 0146-0404
CID: 2754802
Neurons in area LIP differentially encode saccadic targets and visual distractors [Meeting Abstract]
Platt, ML; Glimcher, PW
ISI:A1996TX39702200
ISSN: 0146-0404
CID: 2754792
NEURONS OF THE BASAL GANGLIA CODE THE POSITION OF THE EYE POSTSACCADICALLY [Meeting Abstract]
CHUKOSKIE, L; HANDEL, A; GLIMCHER, PW
ISI:A1995QM91501657
ISSN: 0146-0404
CID: 2754782
Effects of low-frequency stimulation of the superior colliculus on spontaneous and visually guided saccades
Glimcher, P W; Sparks, D L
1. The first experiment of this study determined the effects of low-frequency stimulation of the monkey superior colliculus on spontaneous saccades in the dark. Stimulation trains, subthreshold for eliciting short-latency fixed-vector saccades, were highly effective at biasing the metrics (direction and amplitude) of spontaneous movements. During low-frequency stimulation, the distribution of saccade metrics was biased toward the direction and amplitude of movements induced by suprathreshold stimulation of the same collicular location. 2. Low-frequency stimulation biased the distribution of saccade metrics but did not initiate movements. The distribution of intervals between stimulation onset and the onset of the next saccade did not differ significantly from the distribution of intervals between an arbitrary point in time and the onset of the next saccade under unstimulated conditions. 3. Results of our second experiment indicate that low-frequency stimulation also influenced the metrics of visually guided saccades. The magnitude of the stimulation-induced bias increased as stimulation current or frequency was increased. 4. The time course of these effects was analyzed by terminating stimulation immediately before, during, or after visually guided saccades. Stimulation trains terminated at the onset of a movement were as effective as stimulation trains that continued throughout the movement. No effects were observed if stimulation ended 40-60 ms before the movement began. 5. These results show that low-frequency collicular stimulation can influence the direction and amplitude of spontaneous or visually guided saccades without initiating a movement. These data are compatible with the hypothesis that the collicular activity responsible for specifying the horizontal and vertical amplitude of a saccade differs from the type of collicular activity that initiates a saccade.
PMID: 8463820
ISSN: 0022-3077
CID: 199352
Representation of averaging saccades in the superior colliculus of the monkey
Glimcher, P W; Sparks, D L
We tested the hypothesis that averaging saccades occur when two different saccades are prepared and executed simultaneously. The activity of saccade-related burst neurons (SRBNs) in the primate superior colliculus was recorded while monkeys made both non-averaging saccades to single targets and averaging saccades which directed the gaze between two simultaneously presented visual targets. For movements of comparable direction and amplitude, the activity measured during averaging and non-averaging saccades was statistically indistinguishable. These results are not consistent with the hypothesis that averaging saccades result from the simultaneous execution of two different saccades at the level of the collicular SRBNs. Instead, these findings indicate that averaging saccades are represented as single intermediate movements within the topographically organized map of these collicular cells.
PMID: 8224068
ISSN: 0014-4819
CID: 199362