Searched for: in-biosketch:yes
person:sullir06
Thermal aspects of parenting
Chapter by: Leon, Michael; Coopersmith, Robert; Beasley, Laura J; Sullivan, Regina M
in: Mammalian parenting: Biochemical, neurobiological, and behavioral determinants by Krasnegor, Norman A [Eds]
New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press, 1990
pp. 400-415
ISBN: 0-19-505600-0
CID: 4669
Norepinephrine and learning-induced plasticity in infant rat olfactory system
Sullivan, R M; Wilson, D A; Leon, M
Postnatal olfactory learning produces both a conditioned behavioral response and a modified olfactory bulb neural response to the learned odor. The present report describes the role of norepinephrine (NE) on both of these learned responses in neonatal rat pups. Pups received olfactory classical conditioning training from postnatal days (PN) 1-18. Training consisted of 18 trials with an intertrial interval of 24 hr. For the experimental group, a trial consisted of a pairing of unconditioned stimulus (UCS, stroking/tactile stimulation) and the conditioned stimulus (CS, odor). Control groups received either only the CS (Odor only) or only the UCS (Stroke only). Within each training condition, pups were injected with either the NE beta-receptor agonist isoproterenol (1, 20, or 4 mg/kg), the NE beta-receptor antagonist propranolol (10, 20, 40 mg/kg), or saline 30 min prior to training. On day 20, pups received one of the following tests: (1) behavioral conditioned responding, (2) injection with 14C-2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) and exposed to the CS odor, or (3) tested for olfactory bulb mitral/tufted cell single-unit responses to the CS odor. The results indicated that training with either: (1) Odor-Stroke-Saline, (2) Odor-Stroke-Isoproterenol-Propranolol, or (3) Odor only-Isoproterenol (2 mg/kg) was sufficient to produce a learned behavioral odor preference, enhanced uptake of 14C-2-DG in the odor-specific foci within the bulb, and a modified output signal from the bulb as measured by single-cell recordings of mitral/tufted cells. Moreover, propranolol injected prior to Odor-Stroke training blocked the acquisition of both the learned behavior and olfactory bulb responses. Thus, NE is sufficient and may be necessary for the acquisition of both learned olfactory behavior and olfactory bulb responses
PMCID:1885972
PMID: 2585063
ISSN: 0270-6474
CID: 140375
Associative Processes in Early Olfactory Preference Acquisition: Neural and Behavioral Consequences
Sullivan RM; Wilson DA; Leon M
Acquisition of behavioral conditioned responding and learned odor preferences during olfactory classical conditioning in rat pups requires forward or simultaneous pairings of the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned stimulus (US). Other temporal relationships between the CS and US do not usually result in learning. The present study examined the influence of this CS-US relationship upon the neural olfactory bulb modifications that are acquired during early classical conditioning. Wistar rat pups were trained from Postnatal Days (PN) 1-18 with either forward (odor overlapping temporally with reinforcing stroking) or backward (stroking followed by odor) CS-US pairings. On PN 19, pups received either a behavioral odor preference test to the odor CS or an injection of (14)C 2-DG and exposure to the odor CS, or olfactory bulb single unit responses were recorded in response to exposure to the odor CS. Only pups that received forward presentations of the CS and US exhibited both a preference for the CS and modified olfactory bulb neural responses to the CS. These results, then, suggest that the modified olfactory bulb neural responses acquired during classical conditioning are guided by the same temporal constraints as those which govern the acquisition of behavioral conditioned responses
PMCID:1892210
PMID: 17572798
ISSN: 0889-6313
CID: 78565
Behavioral and neural correlates of postnatal olfactory conditioning: II. Respiration during conditioning
Do, J T; Sullivan, R M; Leon, M
PMCID:1911207
PMID: 3169383
ISSN: 0012-1630
CID: 3798412
Physical stimulation reduces the brain temperature of infant rats
Sullivan, R M; Wilson, D A; Leon, M
Previous work indicated that physical stimulation, such as that which mimics the stimulation pups receive from the dam, reduces pup body temperature. The present paper reports that the body and brain temperature of 5-day-old pups covaried under steady-state thermal conditions, cold exposure, and warmth exposure (Expt. 1) suggesting that body thermoregulatory mechanisms may also regulate brain temperature. Indeed, physically stimulating pups decreased brain temperature in the neocortex (Expt. 2) and the olfactory bulb (Expt. 3). The mechanism for this brain temperature decrease appears to be an increase in ventilatory heat exchange, the same mechanism responsible for the decrease in body temperature. Pups increased respiration during stimulation, thereby increasing air flow to the lungs where convective and evaporative heat exchange occurred. Indeed, stimulating pups in a high-humidity environment blocked the decrease in brain temperature (Expt. 4). The ability of physical stimulation to decrease brain temperature appears to be limited to neonatal pups, as 10-, 15-, and 20-day-old pups did not exhibit a brain temperature decrease in response to stimulation (Expt. 5)
PMCID:1892163
PMID: 3371556
ISSN: 0012-1630
CID: 140376
Reinforcers in infancy: classical conditioning using stroking or intra-oral infusions of milk as UCS
Sullivan, R M; Hall, W G
Six-day-old rats received 20 forward pairings of an odor-conditioned stimulus (CS) with one of two unconditioned stimuli (UCS); 1) intra-oral milk infusions or 2) stroking with a sable-hair brush. These UCS's produce a common general response of increased behavioral activity, but different specific behaviors. For each UCS, additional pups received backward pairings of the CS and UCS, random pairings of the CS and UCS, CS only, UCS only, or no stimuli. Four hours later, pups received a two-odor choice test to assess the development of an odor preference and a CS-only test to assess the acquisition of conditioned responding (CR). The results of the two-odor choice test indicated that for both UCS's only forward pairings of the CS and UCS resulted in an odor preference. Similarly, the CS-only test showed that only forward pairings of the CS and either UCS were effective in producing CR's; pups that received forward pairings exhibited increased behavioral activity during presentations of the CS, which is an unconditioned response (UCR) to both UCS's. Only the forward paired CS-milk UCS group exhibited increased mouthing and probing during the CS only test; these are UCR's that occur to milk infusions but infrequently to the stroking UCS. These results demonstrate the development of similar conditioned odor preferences using behaviorally activating UCS's, but CR's which are specific to the form of the UCR.
PMID: 3371554
ISSN: 0012-1630
CID: 3798432
Behavioral and neural correlates of postnatal olfactory conditioning: I. Effect of respiration on conditioned neural responses
Sullivan, R M; Wilson, D A; Kim, M H; Leon, M
Following olfactory classical conditioning, infant rats exhibit a preference for the conditioned odor and exhibit enhanced uptake of focal 14C 2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) within the olfactory bulb. The present experiments assessed the role of respiration on the expression of the enhanced 2-DG uptake response. Pups were conditioned from postnatal day (PN) 1-18 with an olfactory stimulus paired with a reinforcing tactile stimulus which mimics maternal contact (Odor-Stroke). Control pups received odor only or tactile stimulation only. On PN 19, pups received 1 of 3 tests: 1) a two-odor choice test, 2) an odor/2-DG test with normal respiration allowed, or 3) an odor/2-DG test with respiration experimentally controlled. The results indicated that: 1) Odor-Stroke pups learned the conditioned odor preference, 2) Odor-Stroke, normally respiring pups exhibited enhanced olfactory bulb 2-DG uptake when compared to control pups. No difference in respiration rate was detected between groups in normally respiring pups. 3) Odor Stroke pups whose breathing was experimentally controlled exhibited enhanced olfactory bulb 2-DG uptake when compared to control pups with an identical number of respirations. Together, these results demonstrate that modified respiration during testing is not required for the expression of a modified olfactory bulb response to learned attractive odors. Therefore, the data suggest that the olfactory system itself is modified by early learning
PMID: 3237818
ISSN: 0031-9384
CID: 140377
One-trial olfactory learning enhances olfactory bulb responses to an appetitive conditioned odor in 7-day-old rats
Sullivan, Regina M; Leon, Michael
Assessed the expression of a conditioned odor preference and focal uptake of [-1-4C]2-deoxyglucose (2-DG) within the olfactory bulb (OB) of neonatal rat pups that had undergone a single olfactory classical conditioning trial. At 6 days of age, Ss were simultaneously exposed for 10 min to an odor (peppermint) and to a reinforcing tactile stimulation similar to that received from the dam. Three control groups received only the odor, only the stimulation, or neither of these stimuli. The next day, Ss were either assessed for differential OB activity using the 2-DG technique or tested for their olfactory preference behavior. Ss that received simultaneous odor and tactile stimulation exhibited an attraction to the conditioned odor in the 2-odor choice test; such Ss had greater focal 2-DG uptake in the OB glomeruli that were responsive to the odor than Ss in all other groups.
PSYCH:1988-28796-001
ISSN: n/a
CID: 78614
Single-unit analysis of postnatal olfactory learning: modified olfactory bulb output response patterns to learned attractive odors
Wilson, D A; Sullivan, R M; Leon, M
Neonatal rats learn to approach odors associated with stimulation normally provided by their mother. The present report describes changes in olfactory bulb single-unit activity following olfactory learning in young rats. Rat pups were exposed from postnatal day 1 to 18 to either (1) peppermint-scented air while receiving tactile stimulation (Pepp-Stroked), (2) peppermint-scented air with no tactile stimulation (Pepp-Only), (3) clean air and tactile stimulation (Stroked-Only), or (4) clean air and no tactile stimulation (Naive). On day 19, single-unit activity was recorded from mitral/tufted cells in urethane-anesthetized, freely breathing pups in response to either peppermint or a novel orange odor. Mitral/tufted cell response patterns to peppermint were significantly altered in Pepp-Stroked animals compared to control pups. Peppermint exposure alone, not associated with tactile stimulation (Pepp-Only), did not affect subsequent single-cell response patterns to that odor. In addition, the modification of response patterns was specific to peppermint and was not associated with a change in respiration rate. Furthermore, Pepp-Stroked pups had a relative behavioral preference for peppermint on day 19 compared to control pups. These results demonstrate that postnatal olfactory learning selectively modifies the subsequent response patterns of olfactory bulb output cells to the attractive odor. Furthermore, these results indicate that the initial coding of an odor's attractive value occurs within the olfactory bulb
PMCID:1892184
PMID: 3668621
ISSN: 0270-6474
CID: 140378
Neural and behavioral plasticity induced by early olfactory learning
Chapter by: Leon, Michael; Coopersmith, Robert; Lee, Suzanne; Sullivan, Regina M; Wilson, Donald A; Woo, Cynthia C
in: Perinatal development: A psychobiological perspective by Krasnegor, Norman A [Eds]
San Diego, CA, US: Academic Press, 1987
pp. 145-167
ISBN: 0-12-445910-2
CID: 4670